Témata prací (Výběr práce)Témata prací (Výběr práce)(verze: 368)
Detail práce
   Přihlásit přes CAS
Essays on Decision Making under Stress
Název práce v češtině: Essays on Decision Making under Stress
Název v anglickém jazyce: Essays on Decision Making under Stress
Klíčová slova: behaviorální ekonomie, experimenty, stres, riziková averze, stádové chování
Klíčová slova anglicky: behavioral economics, experiments, stress, risk-aversion, herding behavior
Akademický rok vypsání: 2010/2011
Typ práce: disertační práce
Jazyk práce: angličtina
Ústav: Institut ekonomických studií (23-IES)
Vedoucí / školitel: doc. PhDr. Michal Bauer, Ph.D.
Řešitel: skrytý - zadáno a potvrzeno stud. odd.
Datum přihlášení: 11.11.2015
Datum zadání: 11.11.2015
Datum potvrzení stud. oddělením: 11.11.2015
Datum a čas obhajoby: 22.06.2016 17:30
Datum odevzdání elektronické podoby:21.04.2016
Datum proběhlé obhajoby: 22.06.2016
Oponenti: Mgr. Michal Skořepa, Ph.D.
  Ing. René Levínský, Ph.D.
  Prof. Maroš Servátka, Ph.D.
 
Seznam odborné literatury
Abeler, J., Falk, A., Goette, L., & Huffman, D. (2011). Reference Points and Effort Provision. American Economic Review, 101(2), 470–492. doi:doi: 10.1257/aer.101.2.470
Achtziger, A., & Alós-Ferrer, C. (2014). Fast or Rational? A response-times study of Bayesian updating. Management Science, 60(4), 923–938. doi:10.1287/mnsc.2013.1793
Aizer, A., & Currie, J. (2014). The intergenerational transmission of inequality: Maternal disadvantage and health at birth. Science, 344(6186), 856–861. doi:10.1126/science.1251872
Allen, A. P., Kennedy, P. J., Cryan, J. F., Dinan, T. G., & Clarke, G. (2014). Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 38, 94–124. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Kautz, T. D. (2011). Personality Psychology and Economics. NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES, (16822).
Andersen, S., Ertaç, S., Gneezy, U., Hoffman, M., & List, J. A. (2011). Stakes Matter in Ultimatum Games. American Economic Review, 101(December), 3427–3439.
Andersen, S., Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Rutström, E. E. (2006). Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics, 9(4), 383–405. doi:10.1007/s10683-006-7055-6
Anderson, L. R., & Holt, C. A. (1997). Information cascades in the laboratory. The American Economic Review, 87(5), 847–862. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2951328
Ariely, D., Gneezy, U., Loewenstein, G., & Mazar, N. (2009). Large Stakes and Big Mistakes. Review of Economic Studies, 76(2), 451–469. doi:10.1111/j.1467-937X.2009.00534.x
Arnsten, A. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–22. doi:10.1038/nrn2648
Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, Leadership, and Men (pp. 177–190). Carnegie Press. doi:citeulike-article-id:975612
Avery, C. N., & Zemsky, P. (1998). Multidimensional Uncertainty and Herd Behavior in Financial Markets. American Economic Review, 88(4), 724–748. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:aea:aecrev:v:88:y:1998:i:4:p:724-48
Baddeley, M. (2010). Herding, social influence and economic decision-making: socio-psychological and neuroscientific analyses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 365(1538), 281–90. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0169
Baddeley, M., Burke, C. J., Schultz, W., & Tobler, P. N. (2010). Impacts of personality on herding in financial decision-making. Cambridge Working Papers in Economics (Vol. No. 1006). Cambridge: Faculty of Economics, University of Cambridge. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/p/cam/camdae/1006.html
Baddeley, M., Pillas, D., Christopoulos, Y., Schultz, W., & Tobler, P. N. (2007). Herding And Social Pressure In Trading Tasks: A Behavioural Analysis. Cambridge Working Papers in Economics, No. 730. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:cam:camdae:0730
Banerjee, A. V. (1992). A Simple Model of Herd Behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(3), 797–817. Retrieved from <Go to ISI>://A1992JJ86300001
Baum, A., & Grunberg, N. E. (1997). Measuring Stress Hormones. In S. Cohen, R. C. Kessler, & L. U. Gordon (Eds.), Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists (pp. 175–192). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Baum, A., Grunberg, N. E., & Singer, J. E. (1982). The use of psychological and neuroendocrinological measurements in the study of stress. Health Psychology, 1(3), 217–236. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.1.3.217
Baur, D. G., & McDermott, T. K. (2010). Is gold a safe haven? International evidence. Journal of Banking & Finance, 34(8), 1886–1898. doi:10.1016/j.jbankfin.2009.12.008
Becker, A., Deckers, T., Dohmen, T., Falk, A., & Kosse, F. (2012). The Relationship Between Economic Preferences and Psychological Personality Measures. Annual Review of Economics, 4(1), 453–478. doi:10.1146/annurev-economics-080511-110922
Becker, G. M., DeGroot, M., & Marschak, J. (1964). Measuring utility by a single response sequential method. Behavioral Science, 9(3), 226–232.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
Bikhchandani, S., Hirshleifer, D. A., & Welch, I. (1992). A Theory of Fads, Fashion, Custom, and Cultural Change as Informational Cascades. Journal of Political Economy, 100(5), 992–1026. Retrieved from <Go to ISI>://A1992JR33100005
Bikhchandani, S., Hirshleifer, D., & Welch, I. (1998). Learning from the Behavior of Others: Conformity, Fads, and Informational Cascades. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(3), 151–170. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:aea:jecper:v:12:y:1998:i:3:p:151-70
Bikhchandani, S., & Sharma, S. (2000). Herd behavior in financial markets. IMF Staff Papers, 279–310.
Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., … Fortunato, C. K. (2011). Executive Functions in Early Childhood. Child Development, 82(6), 1970–1984. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x.Salivary
Blandini, F., Martignoni, E., Sances, E., Bono, G., & Nappi, G. (1995). Combined response of plasma and platelet catecholamines to different types of short-term stress. Life Sciences, 56(13), 1113–1120.
Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Weel, B. ter. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43(4), 972–1059. Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/papers/w13810.ack
Brand, M., Fujiwara, E., Borsutzky, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2005). Decision-Making Deficits of Korsakoff Patients in a New Gambling Task With Explicit Rules: Associations With Executive Functions. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 267–277. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.267
Brock, W. A., & Durlauf, S. N. (2001). Interactions-based models. Handbook of Econometrics, 5, 3297–3380.
Buckert, M., Oechssler, J., & Schwieren, C. (2014). Imitation under stress. University of Heidelberg Discussion Paper Series, (556).
Buckert, M., Schwieren, C., Kudielka, B. M., & Fiebach, C. J. (2014). Acute stress affects risk taking but not ambiguity aversion. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8(May), 82. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00082
Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Baddeley, M., & Schultz, W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(32), 14431–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003111107
Cadsby, C., Song, F., & Tapon, F. (2009). The impact of risk aversion and stress on the incentive effect of performance pay. Unpublished Paper, University of Guelph. Retrieved from http://www.uoguelph.ca/economics/sites/uoguelph.ca.economics/files/2009_12.pdf
Callen, M., Long, J. D., & Sprenger, C. (2014). Violence and Risk Preference: Experimental Evidence. American Economic Review, 104(1), 123–148. doi:10.1257/aer.104.1.123
Cameron, L., Erkal, N., Gangadharan, L., & Zhang, M. (2015). Cultural integration : Experimental evidence of convergence in immigrants ’ preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 111, 38–58. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2014.11.012
Cao, H. H., & Hirshleifer, D. A. (2000). Conversation, Observational Learning, and Informational Cascades. Dice Center Working Paper, No. 2001-5. doi:10.2139/ssrn.267770
Capra, C. M., Jiang, B., Engelmann, J. B., & Berns, G. S. (2013). Can personality type explain heterogeneity in probability distortions? Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 6(3), 151–166. doi:10.1037/a0033708
Cassar, A., Andrew, H., & von Kessler, C. (2011). Trust, risk, and time preferences after a natural disaster: experimental evidence from Thailand. Mimeo.
Clow, A. (2001). The physiology of stress. In F. Jones & J. Bright (Eds.), Stress, myth, theory, and research (pp. 47–61). Harlow: Pearson Education.
Coates, J. M., & Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(16), 6167–72. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704025105
Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E., & Maréchal, M. A. (2015). Evidence for Countercyclical Risk Aversion : An Experiment with Financial Professionals. American Economic Review, 105(2), 860–885. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.20131314
Cornelisse, S., van Ast, V. A., Haushofer, J., Seinstra, M. S., Kindt, M., & Joëls, M. (2013). Time-dependent effect of hydrocortisone administration on intertemporal choice. Mimeo. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2294189
Costa, J. P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal Personality Assessment in Clinical Practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5–13. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WYX-46VD04F-2/2/430de88263303f8e481e3dac5a7a52b7
Cox, J. C., Sadiraj, V., & Schmidt, U. (2015). Paradoxes and mechanisms for choice under risk. Experimental Economics, 18, 215–250. doi:10.1007/s11294-013-9452-x
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474. Retrieved from http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/aea/jel/2009/00000047/00000002/art00003
Currie, J. (2011). Inequality at Birth: Some Causes and Consequences. American Economic Review, 101(3), 1–22. doi:10.1257/aer.101.3.1
Deck, C., Lee, J., & Reyes, J. (2010). Personality and the Consistency of Risk Taking Behavior: Experimental Evidence. Chapman Univesity Working Paper, (10-17).
Deck, C., Lee, J., Reyes, J., & Rosen, C. (2012). Risk-Taking Behavior: An Experimental Analysis of Individuals and Dyads. Southern Economic Journal, 79(2), 277–299. doi:10.4284/0038-4038-2010.026
Dedovic, K., Rexroth, M., Wolff, E., Duchesne, A., Scherling, C., Beaudry, T., … Pruessner, J. C. (2009). Neural correlates of processing stressful information: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research, 1293, 49–60.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute Stressors and Cortisol Responses: A Theoretical Integration and Synthesis of Laboratory Research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–391. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0033290904009451
Dohmen, T. (2008). Do professionals choke under pressure? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 65(3-4), 636–653. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2005.12.004
Dohmen, T., & Falk, A. (2011). Performance pay and multidimensional sorting: Productivity, preferences, and gender. The American Economic Review, 101(2), 556–590.
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2010). Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability? The American Economic Review, 100(3), 1238–1260. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1146778
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Individual Risk Attitudes: Measurement, Determinants and Behavioral Consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9(3), 522–550. doi:10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x
Driskell, J. E., & Salas, E. (1991). Group decision making under stress. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(3), 473–478. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/apl/76/3/473/
Eckel, C. C., El-Gamal, M. A., & Wilson, R. K. (2009). Risk loving after the storm: A Bayesian-Network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 69(2), 110–124. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2007.08.012
Elwér, S., Harryson, L., Bolin, M., & Hammarström, A. (2013). Patterns of gender equality at workplaces and psychological distress. PloS One, 8(1), e53246. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053246
Engert, V., Efanov, S. I., Duchesne, A., Vogel, S., Corbo, V., & Pruessner, J. C. (2013). Differentiating anticipatory from reactive cortisol responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(8), 1328–1337. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.018
Engert, V., Plessow, F., Miller, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Singer, T. (2014). Cortisol increase in empathic stress is modulated by emotional closeness and observation modality. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 45, 192–201. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.005
Eurofound, & EU-OSHA. (2014). Psychosocial risks in Europe Prevalence and strategies for prevention. doi:10.2806/70971
Everly, G. S., & Lating, J. M. (2013a). A Clinical Guide to the Treatment of the Human Stress Response. New York, New York, USA: Springer. doi:10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
Everly, G. S., & Lating, J. M. (2013b). The Anatomy and Physiology of the Human Stress Response. In A Clinical Guide to the Treatment of the Human Stress Response (pp. 17–51). doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-5538-7
Falk, A., Menrath, Kupio, & Siegrist, M.-A. (2006). Unfair pay and Stress. mimeo. Bonn: University of Bonn.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–60. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Feige, C. (2005). Stress und Motivation : Die Auswirkung unfairer Behandlung am Arbeitsplatz. University of Bonn.
Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-TREE: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:kap:expeco:v:10:y:2007:i:2:p:171-178
Fletcher, T. D., Major, D. A., & Davis, D. D. (2008). The interactive relationship of competitive climate and trait competitiveness with workplace attitudes, stress, and performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29(7), 899–922. doi:10.1002/job.503
Foley, P., & Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Human hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 91–96. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.010
Ganster, D. C., & Rosen, C. C. (2013). Work Stress and Employee Health: A Multidisciplinary Review. Journal of Management, 39(5), 1085–1122. doi:10.1177/0149206313475815
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2008). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind. New York: W. W. Norton & Company; Third Edition.
Gilbert, T., & Kogan, S. (2005). Learning From The Skills Of Others: Experimental Evidence. GSIA Working Papers. Retrieved from https://student-3k.tepper.cmu.edu/GSIADOC/WP/2006-E25.pdf
Goette, L., Bendahan, S., Thoresen, J., Hollis, F., & Sandi, C. (2015). Stress pulls us apart : Trait anxiety modulates self-confidence under stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 54, 115–123. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.019
Goh, J., Pfeffer, J., & Zenios, S. a. (2015). The Relationship Between Workplace Stressors and Mortality and Health Costs in the United States. Management Science. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2014.2115
Goldberg, L. R. (2010). International Personality Item Pool: A Scientific Collaboratory for the Development of Advanced Measures of Personality Traits and Other Individual Differences. Retrieved from http://ipip.ori.org/
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. G. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(1), 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007
Goldstein, D. S., & McEwen, B. S. (2002). Allostasis, Homeostats, and the Nature of Stress. Stress, 5(1), 55–58. doi:doi:10.1080/102538902900012345
Greene, W. H. (2003). Econometric Analysis. (P. Education, Ed.)Journal of the American Statistical Association (Vol. 97). Prentice Hall. doi:10.1198/jasa.2002.s458
Greiner, B. (2004). An online recruitment system for economic experiments. In K. Kremer & V. Macho (Eds.), Forschung und wissenschaftliches Rechnen GWDG Bericht. Göttingen: Gesellschaft fur Wissenschaftliche Datenverarbeitung.
Grether, D. M. (1980). Bayes’ Rule as a Descriptive Model: The Representativeness Heuristic. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 95(3), 537–557.
Guiso, L., & Paiella, M. (2008). Risk Aversion, Wealth, and Background Risk. Journal of the European Economic Association, 6(6), 1109–1150.
Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2013). Time Varying Risk Aversion. NBER Working Paper #19284. doi:10.3386/w19284
Hartley, L. R., & Adams, R. G. (1974). Effect of noise on the Stroop test. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 62–66.
Haushofer, J., Cornelisse, S., Seinstra, M., Fehr, E., Joëls, M., & Kalenscher, T. (2013). No effects of psychosocial stress on intertemporal choice. PloS One, 8(11), e78597. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078597
Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862–867. doi:10.1126/science.1232491
Haushofer, J., & Jang, C. (2015). Stress and Temporal Discounting : Do Domains Matter? Mimeo.
Haushofer, J., & Shapiro, J. (2013). Household Response to Income Changes : Evidence from an Unconditional Cash Transfer Program in Kenya. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Heckman, J. J. (2011). Integrating personality psychology into economics. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series, No. 17378. Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/papers/w17378
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54(12), 1389–1398.
Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S., & Kudielka, B. M. (2009). Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 163–171. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026
Hines, E. A., & Brown, G. E. (1936). The cold pressor test for measuring the reactibility of the blood pressure: data concerning 571 normal and hypertensive subjects. American Heart Journal, 11(1), 1–9.
Hirshleifer, D., & Hong Teoh, S. (2003). Herd Behaviour and Cascading in Capital Markets: a Review and Synthesis. European Financial Management, 9(1), 25–66. doi:10.1111/1468-036x.00207
Hockey, G. R. J. (1970). Effect of loud noise on attentional selectivity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 28–36.
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2007). Hogan Personality Inventory Manual, Third Edition. Tulsa, OK: Hogan Assessment Systems.
Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects. The American Economic Review, 92(5), 1644–1655. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3083270
Hott, C. (2009). Herding behavior in asset markets. Journal of Financial Stability, 5(1), 35–56. doi:10.1016/j.jfs.2008.01.004
Huang, J.-H. J., & Chen, Y.-F. Y. (2006). Herding in online product choice. Psychology and Marketing, 23(May 2006), 413–428. doi:10.1002/mar
Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2012). Strong Evidence for Gender Differences in Risk Taking. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 50–58. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.06.007
Charness, G., & Levin, D. (2005). When optimal choices feel wrong: A laboratory study of Bayesian updating, complexity, and affect. American Economic Review, 95(4), 1300–1309.
Chemin, M., Laat, J. De, & Haushofer, J. (2013). Negative Rainfall Shocks Increase Levels of the Stress Hormone Cortisol Among Poor Farmers in Kenya. Mimeo.
Chen, M., Ma, Q., Li, M., & Wang, X. (2010). The Neural and Psychological Basis of Herding in Purchasing Books Online: An Event-Related Potential Study. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(3), 7. doi:doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0142
Choi, N., & Skiba, H. (2015). Institutional Herding in International Markets. Journal of Banking & Finance, 55, 246–259. doi:10.1016/j.jbankfin.2015.02.002
Jackson, D. N. (1983). JPI-R: Jackson Personality Inventory, Revised. Sigma Assesment Systems, Incorporated.
Jennings, J., Bberg, W., Hutcheson, S., Obrist, P., Porges, S., & Turpin, G. (1981). Publication guidelines for heart rate studies in man. Psychophysiology, 18(3), 226–231. Retrieved from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03023.x/abstract
Joëls, M., & Baram, T. Z. (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 459–466.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4), 237. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/80/4/237/
Kajantie, E., & Phillips, D. I. W. (2006). The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(2), 151–178. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002
Kalia, M. (2002). Assessing the economic impact of stress--the modern day hidden epidemic. Metabolism: Clinical and Experimental, 51(6 Suppl 1), 49–53. doi:10.1053/meta.2002.33193
Kandasamy, N., Hardy, B., Page, L., Schaffner, M., Graggaber, J., Powlson, A. S., … Coates, J. M. (2014). Cortisol shifts financial risk preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(9), 3608–13. doi:10.1073/pnas.1317908111
Kaul, A., & Sapp, S. (2006). Y2K fears and safe haven trading of the U.S. dollar. Journal of International Money and Finance, 25(5), 760–779. doi:10.1016/j.jimonfin.2006.04.003
Kemeny, M. E. (2003). The Psychobiology of Stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(4), 124–129. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01246
Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1989). Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: an overview. Neuropsychobiologyy, 22(3), 150–169.
Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2000). Salivary Cortisol. In Encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 3, pp. 379–383). Academia Press.
Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(2), 154–62. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10204967
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier Social Stress Test” - A Tool for Investigating Psychobiological Stress Responses in a Laboratory Setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1-2), 76–81. Retrieved from http://www.karger.com/DOI/10.1159/000119004
Klucharev, V., Hytönen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A., & Fernández, G. (2009). Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron, 61(1), 140–51. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027
Klucharev, V., Munneke, M. A. M., Smidts, A., & Fernández, G. (2011). Downregulation of the posterior medial frontal cortex prevents social conformity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(33), 11934–11940.
Kocher, M. G., & Sutter, M. (2006). Time is money—Time pressure, incentives, and the quality of decision-making. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 61(3), 375–392.
Kudielka, B. M., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(1), 83–98.
Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., & Wüst, S. (2009). Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(1), 2–18. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004
Leder, J., Häusser, J. A., & Mojzisch, A. (2013). Stress and strategic decision-making in the beauty contest game. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1503–11. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.016
Leder, J., Häusser, J. A., & Mojzisch, A. (2015). Exploring the underpinnings of impaired strategic decision-making under stress. Journal of Economic Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2015.05.006
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. a. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 8(2), 75–84. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75
Lighthall, N. R., Mather, M., & Gorlick, M. a. (2009). Acute stress increases sex differences in risk seeking in the balloon analogue risk task. PLoS One, 4(7), e6002. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006002
Lo, A. W., & Repin, D. V. (2002). The Psychophysiology of Real-Time Financial Risk Processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 323–339. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:nbr:nberwo:8508
Lundberg, U. (1993). On the Psychobiology of Stress and Health. (O. Svenson & A. J. Maule, Eds.)Time Pressure and Stress in Human Judgement and Decision Making. New York: Springer.
Malmendier, U., & Nagel, S. (2011). Depression Babies: Do Macroeconomic Experiences Affect Risk Taking? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(1), 373–416. doi:10.1093/qje/qjq004
Maule, J. A., & Edland, A. C. (2000). The Effects of time pressure on human judgment and decision making. In R. Ranyard, W. R. Crozier, & O. Svenson (Eds.), Decision making: cognitive models and explanations. London: Routledge.
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews, 87(3), 873–904. doi:10.1152/physrev.00041.2006
McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: how the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109 Suppl , 17180–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1121254109
Michl, T., Koellinger, P., & Picot, A. O. (2011). In the mood for risk? An experiment on moods and risk preferences. Ludwig-Maximilians University Working Paper Series.
Miller, E. K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(1), 59–65.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
Miller, R., Plessow, F., Kirschbaum, C., & Stalder, T. (2013). Classification criteria for distinguishing cortisol responders from nonresponders to psychosocial stress: evaluation of salivary cortisol pulse detection in panel designs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75(10), 832–40. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000002
Nguyen, Y., & Noussair, C. N. (2014). Risk Aversion and Emotions. Pacific Economic Review, 19(3), 296–312. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2294859
Nicolson, N. (2007). Measurement of cortisol. In L. J. and G. Leucken L. C. (Ed.), Handbook of physiological research methods in health psychology (pp. 37–73). SAGE Publications.
Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(August), 1067–1101. doi:10.1162/qjec.122.3.1067
Oberlechner, T., & Nimgade, A. (2005). Work stress and performance among financial traders. Stress and Health, 21(5), 285–293. doi:10.1002/smi.1063
Pabst, S., Brand, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2013a). Stress and decision making: a few minutes make all the difference. Behavioural Brain Research, 250, 39–45. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.046
Pabst, S., Brand, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2013b). Stress effects on framed decisions: there are differences for gains and losses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(142). doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00142
Park, A., & Sabourian, H. (2011). Herding and contrarian behavior in financial markets. Econometrica, 79(4), 973–1026. Retrieved from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.3982/ECTA8602/abstract
Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (1996). When time is money: Decision behavior under opportunity-cost time pressure. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(2), 131–152. Retrieved from http://faculty.fuqua.duke.edu/~jrb12/bio/Jim/46.pdf
Petzold, A., Plessow, F., Goschke, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Stress reduces use of negative feedback in a feedback-based learning task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124(2), 248–55. doi:10.1037/a0018930
Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2009). Acute Stress Modulates Risk Taking in Financial Decision Making. Psychological Science, 20(3), 278–283.
Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6(November), 157. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00157
Putman, P., Antypa, N., & Crysovergi, P. (2010). Exogenous cortisol acutely influences motivated decision making in healthy young men. Psychopharmacology, 208(2), 257–263. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1725-y
Raafat, R. M., Chater, N., & Frith, C. (2009). Herding in humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 420–428. Retrieved from http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661309001703
Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (2008). Inferences under time pressure: How opportunity costs affect strategy selection. Acta Psychologica, 127, 258–276.
Rogers, R. D., Owen, a M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(20), 9029–9038. doi:0270-6474/99/19209029-10$05.00/0
Roodman, D. (2012). CMP: Stata module to implement conditional (recursive) mixed process estimator. Statistical Software Components.
Salop, S. C. S. (1987). Evaluating uncertain evidence with Sir Thomas Bayes: A note for teachers. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 1(1), 155–159. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/1942953
Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science (New York, N.Y.), 318(5850), 598–602. doi:10.1126/science.1142996
Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How Do Glucocorticoids Influence Stress Responses ? Preparative Actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21(April), 55–89. doi:10.1210/er.21.1.55
Sharma, S., & Bikhchandani, S. (2000). Herd Behavior in Financial Markets - A Review. International Monetary Fund. Retrieved from http://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:imf:imfwpa:00/48
Schipper, B. (2012). Sex hormones and choice under risk. UC Davis Department of Economics Working Paper Series, 12(7).
Schwabe, L., Dalm, S., Schächinger, H., & Oitzl, M. S. (2008). Chronic stress modulates the use of spatial and stimulus-response learning strategies in mice and man. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(3), 495–503. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.015
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(22), 7191–7198. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009
Sinha, R. (2001). How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology, 158(4), 343–359.
Sinha, R. (2008). Chronic Stress, Drug Use, and Vulnerability to Addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1141, 105–130. doi:10.1196/annals.1441.030.Chronic
Skořepa, M. (2010). Decision making: a behavioral economic approach. Palgrave Macmillan.
Starcke, K., & Brand, M. (2012). Decision making under stress: A selective review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1228–1248. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.003
Starcke, K., Wolf, O. T., Markowitsch, H. J., & Brand, M. (2008). Anticipatory stress influences decision making under explicit risk conditions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122(6), 1352–1360. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19045954
Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P., & Eid, M. (1997). MDBF-Mehrdimensionaler Befindlichkeitsfragebogen. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Svenson, O., & Benson, L. (1993). On Experimental Instructions and the Inducement of Time Pressure Behavior. (O. Svenson & A. J. Maule, Eds.)Time pressure and stress in human judgement and decision making. New York: Springer.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3), 411.
Tinghög, G., Andersson, D., Bonn, C., Böttiger, H., Josephson, C., Lundgren, G., … Johannesson, M. (2013). Intuition and cooperation reconsidered. Nature, 498(7452), E1–2; discussion E2–3. doi:10.1038/nature12194
Trautmann, S. T. (2014). Risk taking under stress: the role(s) of self-selection. A comment on Buckert et al. (2014). Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8(July), 197. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00197
United Nations. (2015). The Millennium Development Goals Report. New York, New York, USA. Retrieved from https://visit.un.org/millenniumgoals/2008highlevel/pdf/MDG_Report_2008_Addendum.pdf
Upper, C. (2000). How Safe Was the “Safe Haven” ? Financial Market Liquidity during the 1998 Turbulences. Discussion Paper, Economic Research Group of the Deutsche Bundesbank, 1(February). Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=219132
van den Bos, R., Harteveld, M., & Stoop, H. (2009). Stress and decision-making in humans: performance is related to cortisol reactivity, albeit differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 1449–58. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.016
Vieider, F. M., Lefebvre, M., Bouchouicha, R., Chmura, T., Hakimov, R., Krawczyk, M., & Martinsson, P. (2014). Common components of risk and uncertainty attitudes across contexts and domains: Evidence from 30 countries. Journal of the European Economic Association, 1–32. doi:10.1111/jeea.12102
Vinkers, C. H., Zorn, J. V, Cornelisse, S., Koot, S., Houtepen, L. C., Olivier, B., … Joëls, M. (2013). Time-dependent changes in altruistic punishment following stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1467–75. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.012
von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., & Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychological Science, 23(6), 651–60. doi:10.1177/0956797611431576
von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). The Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G): A new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(4), 514–22. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.004
Voors, M. J., Nillesen, E. E. M., Verwimp, P., Bulte, E. H., Lensink, R., & Soest, D. P. Van. (2012). Violent Conflict and Behavior: A Field Experiment in Burundi. The American Economic Review, 102(2), 941–964. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.102.2.941
Weizsäcker, G. (2010). Do We Follow Others when We Should? A Simple Test of Rational Expectations. American Economic Review, 100(5), 2340–2360. doi:10.1257/aer.100.5.2340
Welch, I. (1992). Sequential Sales, Learning and Cascades. Journal of Finance, 47(2), 695–732.
Yesuf, M., & Bluffstone, R. A. (2009). Poverty, Risk Aversion, and Path Dependence in Low-Income Countries: Experimental Evidence from Ethiopia. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 91(4), 1022–1037. doi:10.1111/j.l467-8276.2009.01307.x
Young, E. a., Abelson, J., & Lightman, S. L. (2004). Cortisol pulsatility and its role in stress regulation and health. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 25, 69–76. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2004.07.001
Zafar, B. (2011). An experimental investigation of why individuals conform. European Economic Review, 55(6), 774–798. doi:10.1016/j.euroecorev.2011.01.002
Zizzo, D. J. (2010). Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 13(1), 75–98. doi:10.1007/s10683-009-9230-z

Předběžná náplň práce
Tato práce se skládá ze tří tématicky propojených experimentálních studií lidského chování za nestandardních podmínek: pod časovou tísní a stresem. V úvodní části argumentuji, proč je pro ekonomy důležité, aby vzali na vědomí výzkum stresu v ekonomii, přičemž pro podporu svých argumentů uvádím několik příkladů ze současné literatury. První článek v kapitole 2 zkoumá vliv časové tísně na individuální náchylnost ke stádovému chování, zatímco zbývající dva články zkoumají účinek akutního stresu na rizikové preference, respektive na stádové chování. Stádové chování je velmi důležitý jev v lidském rozhodování, jelikož v našich každodenních životech jsou různé sociální vlivy velmi časté a nejinak je tomu při ekonomickém rozhodování: jako příklad uveďme obchodníky na finančních trzích, wait-and-see investory, ale také spotřební chování v důsledku módy, módních trendů či top-ten seznamů. Rizikové preference jsou dalším zásadním prvkem, který určuje mnoho důležitých hospodářských jevů, a předpoklad jejich stability je stavebním kamenem mnoha ekonomických teorií.

První článek zkoumá kauzální vliv časového tlaku na stádové chování. Za tímto účelem byl proveden laboratorní experiment, ve kterém každý proband řešil kognitivně jednoduchý úkol pod třemi úrovněmi časové tísně. Po prvním samostatném výkonu pak probandi dostali šanci podívat se na rozhodnutí druhých, a podle toho pak změnit jejich vlastní rozhodnutí, což bylo vzato jako ukazatel stádového chování. Hlavním zjištěním tohoto výzkumu je, že lidé relativně často mění své původní rozhodnutí, nicméně míra tohoto stádového chování se neliší mezi různými úrovněmi časové tísně. Na druhou stranu jiné proměnné implicitně spojené s časovým tlakem statisticky významně predikovaly stádové chování, jako například čas strávený na obrazovce zobrazující rozhodnutí druhých, subjektivní hladina stresu a zvýšení srdečního tepu v průběhu řešení úkolu. Skutečnost, že zvýšení tepové frekvence v průběhu řešení úkolu korelovala se subjektivním stresem, naznačuje, že časová tíseň může být použita jako mírný stresor. Bohužel se srdeční frekvence ukázala pro měření fyziologického stresu poměrně hrubým měřítkem, jelikož se může měnit i v důsledku jiných faktorů, jako například samotné úsilí vyvinuté při úloze nebo fyzický pohyb, a ne stresu. Kromě toho byla zjištěna zajímavá souvislost mezi zvýšením srdeční tepu a rizikovými preferencemi u mužů, což naznačuje existenci vztahu mezi fyziologickým stresem a vztahem k riziku.

Ve druhém článku představujeme navazující experiment, ve kterém bylo 151 probandů vystaveno laboratorní stres-vyvolávající nebo kontrolní proceduře "Trier Social Stress Test" pro skupiny s cílem nalézt kauzální vliv stresu na individuální postoje k riziku. Pro měření postojů k riziku bylo použito standardní externě ověřené experimentální metody. Použitím tří různých veličin zaprvé ukazujeme, že probandi ve intervenční skupině byli pod fyziologickým a psychickým stresem: jejich hodnoty srdečního tepu a kortizolu byly po proceduře výrazně zvýšené a zároveň se cítili hůř a více nervózní, a to v porovnání jak s výchozím stavem tak i s kontrolní skupinou. Naše hlavní zjištění je, že stres braný jako čisté vystavení stresoru (average treatment effect, ATT) tak i fyzilogocký stres (intention-to-treat effect, ITT) statisticky významně zvyšuje averzi k riziku u mužů, když kontrolujeme o jejich pozorovatelné charakteristiky. Velikost efektu odhadujeme na 69% (ATT) a 89% (ITT) velikosti rozdílu v postoji k riziku mezi muži a ženami v kontrolní skupině. U žen pozorujeme efekt ve stejném směru, ale je malý a zanedbatelný.

Třetí článek zkoumá, zda stres způsobuje rozdíly v individuálním stádovém chování. Pro vyvolání stresu používáme stejnou metodu jako v druhém článku, a to Trier Social Stress Test pro skupiny. Na vzorku 140 jedinců za použití stejných tři veličin (tepová frekvence, kortizol a psychologický dotazník na momentální náladu) ukazujeme, že probandi byli skutečně pod stresem. Stádové chování bylo měřeno pomocí úlohy na stanovování pravděpodobnosti jako kvantifikaci reakce na nové podněty, což nám umožnilo mít plnou kontrolu nad informační hodnotou v signálech poskytovanými probandům, přičemž signály pocházely buď ze soukromých nebo veřejných zdrojů. Hlavním výsledkem tohoto výzkumu je, že stádové chování jako relativní váha veřejných signálů v jednotlivém rozhodování se nemění pod stresem. Kromě toho, váha soukromých signálů a přesnost uvedených pravděpodobností se také neliší mezi intervenční a kontrolní skupinou, a to i po kontrole o osobnostní charakteristiky a psychologickou míru konformity. Na druhé straně pozorujeme v úloze chování srovnatelné s jinými studiemi, včetně shluků uvedených pravděpodobností na násobcích pěti a konzervativnosti při stanovování odhadu vyšších nebo nižších pravděpodobností.
Předběžná náplň práce v anglickém jazyce
This dissertation comprises three thematically connected experimental studies of human behavior under non-standard conditions: time-pressure and stress. In the Introduction section I present the argument for why it is important for economists to recognize stress research as a valid part of the research in economics and how it can contribute to the growing knowledge of human behavior in general, including several examples from the literature. The first paper presented in Chapter 2 examines the effect of time pressure on the individual propensity to herd, while the remaining two papers examine the effect of acute stress on risk-preferences and herding behavior, respectively. Herding behavior is a very important phenomenon in human decision making since social influence is very frequent in our lives and economic decisions: consider traders in financial markets, wait-and-see investors, but also purchase behavior due to fads, fashion and top-ten lists. Risk preferences are another essential factor which determines many important economic outcomes, and the assumption of their stability is a building block of many economic theories.
The first article investigates the effect of time pressure on herding behavior. To do so, an experiment was run where subjects solved a cognitively simple task under three levels of time pressure in a within-subject design. After having performed first alone, they were then allowed to look at the decisions of others and according to that, change their own decision, which was taken as an indicator of herding behavior. The main finding is that people did frequently change their original decisions, but the rate of doing so was not different under the different levels of time pressure. Nevertheless, other variables implicitly associated with time pressure were significant as predictors of herding behavior, such as the time spent on the screen showing the decisions of others, reported subjective levels of stress and the increase in heart rate during the solution of the task. The fact that the increased heart-rate during the solution of the task correlated with the subjective levels of stress suggests time pressure can be used as a mild stressor. However heart rate is a rather crude measure of physiological stress as it can rise due to other factors, such as effort or simple movement, and not stress, and as a single measure of stress is not satisfactory. We also observe an interesting correlation between heart-rate increases and risk-preferences of men which suggests that there may be a relationship between physiological stress and risk-taking behavior.
In the second article we report on an experiment where we exposed 151 subjects to an efficient laboratory stress-inducing or a control procedure - the Trier Social Stress Test for Groups - in order to find the causal effect of stress on individual risk-attitudes. As a risk measure we used a standard externally validated multiple-price list method. Using three different measures, we first show that the subjects in the treatment-stressed group were both physiologically and psychologically stressed: their heart-rate and cortisol levels increased while they felt worse and more nervous compared to the baseline and to the control group. Our main result is that for men, the exposure to a stressor (intention-to-treat effect, ITT) and the exogenously induced psychosocial stress (the average treatment effect on the treated, ATT) significantly increase risk aversion when controlling for their personal characteristics. The estimated treatment difference in certainty equivalents is equivalent to 69% (ITT) and 89% (ATT) of the gender-difference in the control group. The effect on women goes in the same direction, but is weaker and insignificant.
The third article examines whether stress causes differences in individual herding behavior. To impose stress we employ the same methodology as in the second article, the Trier Social Stress Test for Groups, on a sample of 140 subjects and show using the same three measures as in the previous chapter (heart-rate, cortisol and mood questionnaire) that subjects were indeed stressed. Herding behavior was measured in a Bayesian updating task that allowed for full control over the information provided to subjects either from private or public sources. The main result is that herding behavior as a relative weight of public signals in individual decision making does not change under stress. Apart from that, the weight of private signals and the precision of the stated probabilities were also not different between the treatment and control groups, even after controlling for personality characteristics and the subjects' psychological measure of conformity. On the other hand, we observe updating behavior comparable with other studies, including clusters of stated probabilities on multiples of five and conservatism.
 
Univerzita Karlova | Informační systém UK